Why the Secret to Language May Be Hiding in Your Birdcage
The Bengalese finch is what happens when for 250 years,
people breed the white-rumped munia for its snowy plumage. It’s paler than its
wild counterpart, more social. And although its genetic architects (bird
breeders) weren’t selecting for singing skills, the Bengalese finch has a farmore complex, varied song.
That last bit has some fascinating possible implications, concerning how we
developed our own unique speaking abilities.
The capacity for complex communication requires a wide
variety of skills. You don’t just need to be able to recognize and respond to certain
cues; in order to parse the meanings of new or unfamiliar cues, you also need
to be able to guess at the other party’s intent.
This is not something most wild animals have the bandwidth to
do. For instance, wild foxes and wolves can’t recognize what it means when a
person points to something. No amount of training will pass on the
understanding. However, domesticated animals are the result of many generations
of breeding that selected for sociability, among other traits. So maybe it’s no
surprise that domesticated foxes, dogs, and even formerly domesticated dogs who
have gone feral all have the capacity to learn pointing behaviors.
It’s possible that the same might be true for the Bengalese
finch—and us. Unlike our feathered friends, our development into our present species
wasn’t masterminded by some other creature, but it has been argued that the
overall process was much the same. As selection pressures started to favor less
aggressive, more sociable individuals, communication skills naturally
increased.
It may, however, be a bit more complicated than that. Keep
in mind, the Bengalese finch was not bred for its excellent social skills, but
for the color of its feathers.
In “Self domestication and the evolution of language,” James Thomas and Simon Kirby
argue that the forces reshaping the finch’s song might be more cultural.
The Bengalese finch and the white-rumped munia both pick up
their song from others of their species, learning best from an in-person (in-bird?)
demonstration. In the wild, the white-rumped munia must be constantly on its
guard, and transmitting its song accurately to its young could be a matter of
survival. Fidelity is also key when it comes to identifying other white-rumped
munia by call. But the tame Bengalese finch has adapted to a very different lifestyle,
encountering plentiful food and few threats, and leaving the bird free to get a
little creative with its vocalizations.
Could early humans have developed our own language thanks to
the decreased stresses that came with living in larger groups? Did our first
languages evolve from a sort of jazz freestyle of sounds from the first
generations of proto-humans with a little time on their hands? We are a long,
long way from knowing for sure. But in the meantime, the next time you point
your dog towards a tennis ball, take a moment to thank our ancestors for their truly
impressive wolf-breeding project. From White Fang to Rover, white-rumped munia
to finch, domestication has made our lives easier and more pleasant—even if a
little tamer.
Check out Robb’s new book and more
content at www.bestmindframe.com.
Comments
Post a Comment